March 23, 2025

Maintenance of prior behaviour can enhance cultural selection

Maintenance of prior behaviour can enhance cultural selection
  • 1.

    Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J. & Henrich, J. The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 108, 10918–10925 (2011).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 2.

    Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Feldman, M. W., Chen, K. H. & Dornbusch, S. M. Theory and observation in cultural transmission. Science 218, 19–27 (1982).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 3.

    Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 1149–1157 (2011).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 4.

    Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. Culture and the Evolutionary Process (University of Chicago Press, 1985).


    Google Scholar
     

  • 5.

    Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. W. Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach (Princeton University Press, 1989).


    Google Scholar
     

  • 6.

    Whiten, A., Hinde, R. A., Laland, K. N. & Stringer, C. B. Culture evolves. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 938–948 (2011).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 7.

    Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A. & Laland, K. N. Perspective: Is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of The Origin of Species. Evolution 58, 1–11 (2004).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 8.

    Basalla, G. The Evolution of Technology (Cambridge University Press, 1988).


    Google Scholar
     

  • 9.

    Eriksson, K. & Coultas, J. C. Corpses, maggots, poodles and rats: Emotional selection operating in three phases of cultural transmission of urban legends. J. Cogn. Cult. 14, 1–26 (2014).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 10.

    Nia, H. T. et al. The evolution of air resonance power efficiency in the violin and its ancestors. Proc. R. Soc. Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 471, 20140905 (2015).

    ADS 
    MathSciNet 
    MATH 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 11.

    Vincenti, W. G. Real-world variation-selection in the evolution of technological form: Historical examples. In Technological Innovation as an Evolutionary Process (ed. Ziman, J.) 174–189 (Cambridge University Press, 2000).


    Google Scholar
     

  • 12.

    Morgan, T. J. H. et al. Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nat. Commun. 6, 6029 (2015).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 13.

    Mesoudi, A. & O’Brien, M. J. The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile-point technology I: An experimental simulation. Am. Antiq. 73, 3–28 (2008).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 14.

    Derex, M., Beugin, M.-P., Godelle, B. & Raymond, M. Experimental evidence for the influence of group size on cultural complexity. Nature 503, 389–391 (2013).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 15.

    Mesoudi, A. An experimental simulation of the “copy-successful-individuals” cultural learning strategy: Adaptive landscapes, producer–scrounger dynamics, and informational access costs. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 350–363 (2008).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 16.

    Caldwell, C. A. & Millen, A. E. Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: Is imitation necessary?. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1478–1483 (2009).

    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 17.

    Fay, N., De Kleine, N., Walker, B. & Caldwell, C. A. Increasing population size can inhibit cumulative cultural evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 116, 6726–6731 (2019).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 18.

    Fay, N., Garrod, S. & Roberts, L. The fitness and functionality of culturally evolved communication systems. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 3553–3561 (2008).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 19.

    Kirby, S., Cornish, H. & Smith, K. Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 10681–10686 (2008).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 20.

    Fay, N. & Ellison, T. M. The cultural evolution of human communication systems in different sized populations: Usability trumps learnability. PLoS ONE 8, e71781 (2013).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 21.

    Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G. & Keil, F. C. The hidden structure of overimitation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104, 19751–19756 (2007).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 22.

    Bonaccio, S. & Dalal, R. S. Advice taking and decision-making: An integrative literature review, and implications for the organizational sciences. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 101, 127–151 (2006).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 23.

    Eriksson, K. & Strimling, P. Biases for acquiring information individually rather than socially. J. Evol. Psychol. 7, 309–329 (2009).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 24.

    Hodges, B. H. Rethinking conformity and imitation: Divergence, convergence, and social understanding. Front. Psychol. 5, 726 (2014).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 25.

    Mesoudi, A., Chang, L., Murray, K. & Lu, H. J. Higher frequency of social learning in China than in the West shows cultural variation in the dynamics of cultural evolution. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 282, 20142209 (2015).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 26.

    Yaniv, I. & Kleinberger, E. Advice taking in decision making: Egocentric discounting and reputation formation. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 83, 260–281 (2000).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 27.

    Morin, O., Jacquet, P. O., Vaesen, K. & Acerbi, A. Social information use and social information waste. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200052 (2021).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 28.

    Harrison, R. A. & Whiten, A. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) display limited behavioural flexibility when faced with a changing foraging task requiring tool use. PeerJ 6, e4366 (2018).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 29.

    Gruber, T., Muller, M. N., Reynolds, V., Wrangham, R. & Zuberbühler, K. Community-specific evaluation of tool affordances in wild chimpanzees. Sci. Rep. 1, 128 (2011).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 30.

    Davis, S. J., Vale, G. L., Schapiro, S. J., Lambeth, S. P. & Whiten, A. Foundations of cumulative culture in apes: Improved foraging efficiency through relinquishing and combining witnessed behaviours in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Sci. Rep. 6, 35953 (2016).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 31.

    Nakahashi, W., Wakano, J. Y. & Henrich, J. Adaptive social learning strategies in temporally and spatially varying environments. Hum. Nat. 23, 386–418 (2012).

    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 32.

    McElreath, R. et al. Beyond existence and aiming outside the laboratory: Estimating frequency-dependent and pay-off-biased social learning strategies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 363, 3515–3528 (2008).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 33.

    Kendal, J., Giraldeau, L.-A. & Laland, K. The evolution of social learning rules: Payoff-biased and frequency-dependent biased transmission. J. Theor. Biol. 260, 210–219 (2009).

    ADS 
    MathSciNet 
    PubMed 
    MATH 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 34.

    Ehn, M. & Laland, K. Adaptive strategies for cumulative cultural learning. J. Theor. Biol. 301, 103–111 (2012).

    ADS 
    MathSciNet 
    PubMed 
    MATH 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 35.

    Enquist, M., Eriksson, K. & Ghirlanda, S. Critical social learning: A solution to Rogers’s paradox of nonadaptive culture. Am. Anthropol. 109, 727–734 (2007).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 36.

    Kameda, T. & Nakanishi, D. Does social/cultural learning increase human adaptability?: Rogers’s question revisited. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 242–260 (2003).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 37.

    Rendell, L., Fogarty, L. & Laland, K. N. Rogers’ paradox recast and resolved: Population structure and the evolution of social learning strategies. Evolution 64, 534–548 (2010).

    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 38.

    Rogers, A. R. Does biology constrain culture?. Am. Anthropol. 90, 819–831 (1988).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 39.

    Rogers, S. L. & Fay, N. Stick or switch: A selection heuristic predicts when people take the perspective of others or communicate egocentrically. PLoS ONE 11, e0159570 (2016).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 40.

    McElreath, R. et al. Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 483–508 (2005).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 41.

    Kendal, R. L. et al. Social learning strategies: Bridge-building between fields. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 651–665 (2018).

    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 42.

    Laland, K. N. Social learning strategies. Anim. Learn. Behav. 32, 4–14 (2004).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 43.

    Chudek, M., Heller, S., Birch, S. & Henrich, J. Prestige-biased cultural learning: Bystander’s differential attention to potential models influences children’s learning. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 46–56 (2012).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 44.

    Atkisson, C., O’Brien, M. J. & Mesoudi, A. Adult learners in a novel environment use prestige-biased social learning. Evol. Psychol. 10, 147470491201000320 (2012).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 45.

    Haun, D. B. M., Rekers, Y. & Tomasello, M. Majority-biased transmission in chimpanzees and human children, but not orangutans. Curr. Biol. 22, 727–731 (2012).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 46.

    Lewandowsky, S., Cook, J., Fay, N. & Gignac, G. E. Science by social media: Attitudes towards climate change are mediated by perceived social consensus. Mem. Cognit. 47, 1445–1456 (2019).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 47.

    van den Berg, P., Molleman, L. & Weissing, F. J. Focus on the success of others leads to selfish behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 112, 2912–2917 (2015).

    ADS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 48.

    Chudek, M., Baron, A. S. & Birch, S. Unselective overimitators: The evolutionary implications of children’s indiscriminate copying of successful and prestigious models. Child Dev. 87, 782–794 (2016).

    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 49.

    Tamariz, M. Replication and emergence in cultural transmission. Phys. Life Rev. 30, 47–71 (2019).

    ADS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 50.

    Tamariz, M., Ellison, T. M., Barr, D. J. & Fay, N. Cultural selection drives the evolution of human communication systems. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 281, 20140488 (2014).


    Google Scholar
     

  • 51.

    Rendell, L. et al. Why copy others? Insights from the social learning strategies tournament. Science 328, 208–213 (2010).

    ADS 
    MathSciNet 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    MATH 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 52.

    Schlag, K. H. Why imitate, and if so, how?: A boundedly rational approach to multi-armed bandits. J. Econ. Theory 78, 130–156 (1998).

    MathSciNet 
    MATH 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 53.

    Fay, N., Garrod, S., Roberts, L. & Swoboda, N. The interactive evolution of human communication systems. Cogn. Sci. 34, 351–386 (2010).

    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 54.

    Segovia-Martín, J., Walker, B., Fay, N. & Tamariz, M. Network connectivity dynamics, cognitive biases, and the evolution of cultural diversity in round-robin interactive micro-societies. Cogn. Sci. 44, e12852 (2020).

    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 55.

    Barr, D. J. Establishing conventional communication systems: Is common knowledge necessary?. Cogn. Sci. 28, 937–962 (2004).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 56.

    Simpson, E. H. Measurement of diversity. Nature 163, 688 (1949).

    ADS 
    MATH 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 57.

    Acerbi, A., Jacquet, P. O. & Tennie, C. Behavioral constraints and the evolution of faithful social learning. Curr. Zool. 58, 307 (2012).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 58.

    Enquist, M., Strimling, P., Eriksson, K., Laland, K. & Sjostrand, J. One cultural parent makes no culture. Anim. Behav. 79, 1353–1362 (2010).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 59.

    Kandler, A. & Laland, K. N. An investigation of the relationship between innovation and cultural diversity. Theor. Popul. Biol. 76, 59–67 (2009).

    PubMed 
    MATH 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 60.

    Henrich, J. Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses: The Tasmanian case. Am. Antiq. 69, 197–214 (2004).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 61.

    Kobayashi, Y. & Wakano, J. Y. Evolution of social versus individual learning in an infinite island model. Evolution 66, 1624–1635 (2012).

    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 62.

    Schlag, K. H. Which one should I imitate?. J. Math. Econ. 31, 493–522 (1999).

    MathSciNet 
    MATH 
    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 63.

    Yaniv, I. Receiving other people’s advice: Influence and benefit. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 93, 1–13 (2004).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • 64.

    Harvey, N. & Harries, C. Effects of judges’ forecasting on their later combination of forecasts for the same outcomes. Int. J. Forecast. 20, 391–409 (2004).

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • https://www.nature.com/articles/s41598-021-99340-7